Syllabus Edition
First teaching 2025
First exams 2027
Piaget's Stages of Intellectual Development (AQA A Level Psychology): Revision Note
Exam code: 7182
The sensorimotor stage
The sensorimotor stage spans the ages from birth to around 2 years old and is marked by the child’s body schema and the physical exploration of their environment
Young children in this stage learn via a set of physical schemas, e.g., sucking schema, trajectory schema, which are not reflexes
They are performed intentionally as the child derives pleasure from them (this explains why a baby might hurl their dish from their highchair and then laugh uproariously)
A key marker of this stage of cognitive development is when a baby acquires object permanence, usually around the age of 8 months
Object permanence can be tested using the ‘A-not-B’ task
This involves hiding a toy under location A in front of the child several times in a row and asking the child to retrieve the toy
The toy is then hidden under location B, and if the child continues to look for the toy under location A, they have failed the task (i.e., they have not acquired object permanence)
Passing the ‘A-not-B’ task shows that the child understands that the toy still exists even though it cannot be seen, and their initially reinforced (via several trials) movement to retrieve it from location A can be overridden by their new knowledge that the toy is now in location B

The pre-operational stage
The pre-operational stage is probably the most-researched of all four of Piaget’s stages, as children show the most developmental milestones in this stage, and they are more receptive to taking part in research compared to babies
This stage spans the ages of 2-7 years old and is characterised by the development of increasingly sophisticated schemas, pretend play, anthropomorphism, an understanding of time and the ability to de-centre
Key markers of this stage comprise:
Egocentrism – lacking the ability to see the world from another’s perspective (passing an egocentrism task marks the end of this stage)
Conservation – lacking the ability to appreciate that objects or materials remain the same even when their appearance changes (passing a conservation task marks the end of this stage)
Class inclusion – lacking the ability to classify objects into two or more categories
Conservation
Piaget tested conservation using experimental methodology as follows:
Present equal quantities
Show the child two equal amounts of material side by side (e.g., liquid in identical glasses, equal clay balls, or equal rows of coins)Initial question
Ask: “Does this one have more, does that one have more, or are they the same?”Transform one item
While the child watches, alter the appearance of one material (e.g., pour liquid into a taller container, flatten one clay ball, spread out one row of coins)Repeat the question
Ask the same question again: “Does this one have more, does that one have more, or are they the same?”Evaluate the response
If the child says one now has more/less: Fail – they focus on appearance, not logic
If the child says they’re still the same: Pass – they understand conservation
The conservation task tests a key developmental milestone in children, as to pass it, the child must be able to perform a reverse operation, i.e., by ‘conserving’ the idea that both materials are the same in terms of capacity so that they can go back (in their head) and see them as the same even when the transformed appearance of one of them makes them look as if they have different capacities
Egocentrism
Piaget & Inhelder (1956) tested egocentrism via a test known as the 3 Mountains Task, which runs as follows:
Present the model
Show the child a 3D model landscape with three mountains and features like animals or treesIntroduce the doll
Place a doll on the opposite side of the model, facing the landscape from a different viewpoint than the childAsk the perspective question
Ask the child: “What does the doll see?”
The child selects a picture from several images showing the model from different anglesObserve the response
Young children (around 4 years old) typically choose the image that matches their own view – showing egocentrism
By age 7–8, children more consistently select the doll’s viewpoint – showing reduced egocentrism and improved perspective-taking
The 3 Mountains Task tests a key developmental milestone because to pass it, the child must be able to decentre, which shows more maturity and sophisticated thinking
Class inclusion
Piaget tested class inclusion as follows:
Present the Set
Show the child a picture or model of a set of items, e.g., a bunch of flowersIdentify Subclasses
The set includes two subgroups — for example, five daffodils and three poppiesAsk the Inclusion Question
Ask the child: “Are there more daffodils or more flowers?”Evaluate the Response
If the child answers “more daffodils”, they fail the class inclusion task, showing they do not yet understand that “flowers” includes both daffodils and poppies
If the child answers “more flowers”, they pass, demonstrating an understanding of hierarchical categorisation
The class inclusion task tests a key developmental milestone in children, as to pass it, the child must be able to understand that ‘daffodils’ are a subcategory of the overarching category of ‘flowers’
The concrete operational & the formal operational stages
The concrete operational stage spans the ages 7-11 years and is marked by an understanding of:
the conservation of volume, mass and number
the ability to de-centre
the ability to classify and categorise objects more accurately
Children in this stage can perform mental operations that require logic, e.g., mathematics, up to a certain point, but they may lack a systematic approach to problem-solving
The formal operational stage spans the ages 11+ years and is marked by increasingly systematic and sophisticated mental operations, abstract thought, relativism and the ability to debate and manipulate ideas and principles
Research which investigates Piaget’s stages of intellectual development
Samuel & Bryant (1984) found that by asking 4-8 year-old children only one question in total (after viewing the transformation of volume/mass/number)
Those children made fewer mistakes, even those aged 4 years old thus, Piaget’s stages are not as inflexible as he suggested
Piaget & Inhelder (1956) - The original 3 Mountains study as described above
Hughes (1975) found that fewer children aged 3 to 5 years old fail a test of egocentrism if the procedure changes to include two policeman dolls that a boy doll has to hide from using four interlocking walls, i.e., the children could understand that if the boy stood at point X, then the policemen could not see him
McGarrigle (1974) changed the wording of a standard Piagetian class inclusion task by asking pre-operational children if there were ‘more black cows or more sleeping cows?’ (the standard question was, ‘more black cows or more cows?’)
The cows were placed lying down, they appeared to be sleeping and this resulted in 48% of the children passing the test as opposed to 25% passing the standard Piagetian version

Evaluation of Piaget’s stages of intellectual development
Strengths
Piaget’s stage theory has great application to educational settings, as it sets out the benchmarks for educators and health professionals to use to assess progress and flag any developmental delays that a child may be experiencing
Piaget's stage theory changed the way that researchers think about children, i.e., children are not simply smaller versions of adults
The way children think is fundamentally different from the way that adults think
Limitations
Children do not develop synchronistically, achieving key milestones at the same time, which means that the theory lacks reliability, as it is unlikely to show consistency across age groups and particularly across cultures
Piaget’s original research has had to be modified over the years, as the procedures can themselves constitute confounding variables, e.g., how many children (or even adults) are able to describe a mountainous landscape with ease if they have grown up in a non-mountainous region?
Issues & Debates
Piaget’s theory uses a nomothetic approach, applying universal developmental stages to all children, based on broad patterns of thinking
This ignores individual differences in cognitive development, such as personal experiences, language ability, and cultural background, which are better explained through idiographic approaches
Piaget’s tasks (e.g., the 3 Mountains task) rely on Western cultural knowledge, such as familiarity with mountainous landscapes, which may disadvantage children from non-Western or rural backgrounds
Studies like Dasen (1994) show that cognitive abilities develop differently across cultures, challenging the universality of Piaget’s stages
Worked Example
Here is an example of an A02 question you might be asked on this topic.
AO2: You need to apply your knowledge and understanding, usually referring to the ‘stem’ in order to do so
Professor Connie Servation carried out an experiment to test whether pre-operational children could carry out reverse operations using a modified Piagetian task. She set the probability level at 0.05 to analyse her results.
Q. Explain why Professor Servation chose a 0.05 probability level and what ‘significance’ means in relation to this level.
[3 marks]
Model answer:
Explain why the 0.05 probability level was chosen:
A probability level of 0.05 is commonly used in psychology to balance the risk of making Type I and Type II errors [1 mark]
Explain what significance means:
It means there is less than 5% probability that the results occurred due to chance [1 mark]
If the results are significant at the 0.05 level, this suggests that the findings are unlikely to have happened by chance and that the null hypothesis can be rejected [1 mark]
Unlock more, it's free!
Did this page help you?